Skip to main content
  • Opinion
  • Published:

What is complex about complex disorders?

Abstract

Rather than being polygenic, complex disorders probably represent umbrella terms for collections of conditions caused by rare, recent mutations in any of a large number of different genes.

Introduction

Many papers on the genetics of common human diseases start with the following statement: 'Disease × is a complex, multifactorial disorder'. This rubric has been applied to schizophrenia, autism, depression, asthma, epilepsy, diabetes, rheumatoid arthritis, hypertension, coronary artery disease, obesity, Crohn's disease, Alzheimer's and Parkinson's disease, multiple sclerosis and probably hundreds of other conditions - even dandruff! But what does it mean? It means that the disease is influenced by multiple genetic and environmental factors. Diseases may earn this label if they are clearly heritable and also influenced by environmental factors (as in the case of diabetes), or if the inheritance of genetic liability is not sufficient to predict whether a person will actually develop the disease; that is, there is some probabilistic element to the emergence of the disease state itself (as in many inherited cancers or psychiatric disorders).

Quite apart from whether environmental or stochastic factors are at play, the terms complex and multifactorial are also commonly used to describe the architecture of just the genetic component of disease liability. In these cases, these terms are usually at least implicitly equated with the trait being polygenic; in fact, 'complex', 'multifactorial' and 'polygenic' are commonly used as synonyms. It is important here to make a distinction in how the term polygenic is used: the implication is that the disorder arises in each individual due to the combined effects of a large number of genetic variants [1]. This definition is distinct from a model of genetic heterogeneity, in which many different variants are involved across the population, but where each case is caused by a single variant (or a few variants).

Arguments that the inheritance of a disorder is polygenic usually derive from the observation that, while the disorder might aggregate in families, it does not tend to segregate in ways that are consistent with simple Mendelian inheritance. This is indeed true for the disorders referred to above, for which risk of disease is increased if an individual has a relative with the disease (and increased more, the closer the relative) but where sporadic cases are also common, sometimes forming the majority of cases. The precise values of the relative risks to family members of different degrees of relatedness can be fed into mathematical models of genetic architecture, and are, in many cases, consistent with polygenic inheritance (for example, for schizophrenia [2–4]). In some cases, these kinds of analyses have even been taken as proof that Mendelian inheritance with genetic heterogeneity can be rejected unequivocally as a model of the genetic architecture of the disorder (for example, [3–6]).

The conclusion that many common disorders are polygenic and caused by the aggregate effects in individuals of many common variants [1], along with the development of the HapMap Project [7], laid the foundation for genome-wide association studies (GWAS) designed to identify loci that harbor such common variants [8–10]. Unfortunately, this conclusion is based entirely on circular logic and a number of unfounded assumptions. The most fundamental of these is that the disorder in question represents a single, biologically valid category.

When is a disorder not a disorder?

Analyses of the segregation patterns of a 'disorder' across a population necessarily assume that the disorder in question is a valid category; otherwise, there would be no point in lumping all cases together and calculating things such as heritability and relative risks across the entire population. If the clinical diagnosis of a specific disorder is based on superficial criteria, then this assumption is unlikely to hold.

For example, 'blindness' is not a very informative diagnosis - genetic forms can be caused by cataracts, corneal defects, optic nerve atrophy and various forms of photoreceptor degeneration, such as retinitis pigmentosa (RP) [11]. Each of these, in turn, can arise due to mutations in any of a large number of different genes (over 100 for RP) [12]. Calculating the heritability of blindness or the relative risks to family members, averaged across all of these conditions, would not be a worthwhile or informative endeavor; in fact, the resultant figures would be pretty meaningless. Even within one 'condition', such as RP, such calculations would not be worthwhile as some cases are dominant, others recessive, some X-linked and others autosomal.

'Mental retardation' is another common condition that has very high underlying genetic heterogeneity [13, 14]. In many cases, this heterogeneity is apparent because the condition often arises as part of a distinct and discernible genetic syndrome (causing typical facial morphology, for example). But if we had only the intellectual disability to go on, there would be no way to distinguish these subtypes. If we looked at the inheritance of mental retardation as a whole, it would indeed fit the criteria for a 'complex' disorder. Yet there is no reason to think that most, or indeed any, cases of mental retardation arise due to a polygenic mechanism (that is, in the absence of a reasonably penetrant mutation).

Are 'diabetes', 'schizophrenia' or 'coronary artery disease' any more specific than 'mental retardation' as diagnoses? If two patients had different underlying causes, would we have any way to know this on the basis of their symptom profiles? Is it not possible, even likely, that as with blindness or mental retardation, many different insults could give rise to a similar end-state? This is especially likely if our descriptors are crude. For psychiatric disorders, for example, there is no definitive biomarker, brain scan or blood test that can aid in clinical diagnosis. These disorders are defined on the basis of surface criteria: the patient's behavior and reports of their subjective experience. The diagnostic categories are constantly being debated and the borders between them redefined (for example, [15]). Many patients' diagnoses are fluid over time and two patients can have the same diagnosis without sharing a single symptom in common.

None of this gives much confidence that many disease categories are natural kinds. Treating them as such is thus a massive leap of faith, and as we will see, the empirical evidence has not upheld this belief. GWAS have not uncovered the expected common variants that would explain polygenic inheritance across each of these disorders. By contrast, the identification of rare, individually causal variants in a large number of different genes in different people clearly demonstrates a very high degree of genetic heterogeneity underlying common, complex conditions.

This is especially noteworthy for psychiatric disorders such as autism and schizophrenia, where mutations in over 100 different loci have been found [16–19]. For schizophrenia, genetic heterogeneity had supposedly been definitively rejected on the basis of the observed distribution of familial relative risks [2–4]. As we have seen, this is a circular argument: those numbers only make any sense if the condition is indeed monolithic. As it happens, it is trivial to show that a similar distribution can be generated on the basis of genetic heterogeneity, even by an arbitrary division of cases into different modes of inheritance [18]. Indeed, as originally pointed out by James [20]: there is 'an infinite number of parameter sets ... which lead to the same frequencies in relatives'.

The other argument against genetic heterogeneity is that if rare mutations of high penetrance exist, they should have been found by linkage analysis [4, 21, 22]. This conclusion again rests on several assumptions: that linkage was sought with the right phenotype, that the inconsistent replication of linkage results necessarily means that the large number found are all false positives, and that the level of genetic heterogeneity is low enough that even lumping many different families together into one analysis should still yield real linkage peaks [18, 23]. Again, the data indicate otherwise. Thus, the hypothesis of a polygenic architecture for these disorders arises from the unfounded assumption that they are actually common disorders, as opposed to umbrella terms for a diverse set of very rare genetic conditions that happen to share symptoms. This is, however, just the first of a series of assumptions underlying the search for common variants conferring disease risk.

The theoretical foundation of genome-wide association studies

GWAS are founded on the polygenic model of disease liability, which itself arises from an assertion of breathtaking audacity by the godfather of quantitative genetics, DS Falconer. In an attempt to demonstrate the relevance of quantitative genetics to the study of human disease, Falconer, based on work of others before him (for example, [24]), came up with a nifty solution [25]. Even though disease states are typically all-or-nothing, and even though the actual risk of disease is clearly very discontinuously distributed in the population (being dramatically higher in relatives of affected people, for example), he claimed that it was reasonable to assume that there was something called the underlying liability to the disorder that was actually continuously distributed. This could be converted to a discontinuous distribution by further assuming that only individuals whose burden of genetic variants passed an imagined threshold actually got the disease. To transform discontinuous incidence data (that is, mean rates of disease in various groups, such as people with different levels of genetic relatedness to affected individuals) into mean liability on a continuous scale, it was necessary to further assume that this liability was normally distributed in the population. The corollary is that liability is affected by many genetic variants, each having a small effect [1, 26].

This declaration meant that the statistical techniques developed in animal breeding could supposedly be applied legitimately to the study of human disease. Unfortunately, there is no basis for the underlying assumption of normally distributed liability, nor for the invoked threshold of genetic burden (Box 1; Figure 1).

Figure 1
figure 1

Modeling the genetic components of variance. (a, b) The idea of the multifactorial liability-threshold model is, first, that the actual discontinuous distribution of risk (a) (estimates given for schizophrenia risk to monozygotic twins (MZ) and first and second degree relatives of affected people) can be modeled as a continuous distribution of 'liability' (b). Second, at the extreme end of the normal distribution of 'liability', the cumulative genetic burden of risk alleles suddenly passes a tipping point (from n alleles to n + 1 alleles), triggering pathogenicity (b). (c) Increased risk to relatives can be modeled with a distribution of risk allele load that is shifted to the right. If n is small (0 or 1, for example), then the idea of a threshold of burden makes sense (for example, when there are dominant or recessive alleles). If n is supposed to be in the hundreds or even the thousands, this scenario becomes rather fanciful.

Nevertheless, GWAS have gone ahead on a very large scale for many complex disorders and have produced statistically significant findings. Do these findings validate the assumptions I have claimed are flawed? They do not, at least not necessarily.

What have we learned about complex disorders from GWAS?

GWAS follow a simple design: compare allele frequencies for hundreds of thousands of common variants spread across the genome between large samples of disease cases and controls [10]. If a variant predisposes to the disease, even slightly, this should be apparent as an increased allelic frequency in cases versus controls, given a large enough sample size. This design has been applied to many different complex disorders, with varying degrees of success.

It is important to note that these studies use a sample of single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) to tag variation across the genome (on the basis of blocks of low recombination or linkage disequilibrium (LD)). If a single SNP shows an association with the disease, this does not necessarily mean that that SNP itself is involved, the association could be due to any of the other variants in LD with it. As we will see, these can include tagged rare variants [27]. The goal of these studies is thus not necessarily to identify causal alleles but to point to loci that might harbor them.

By that criterion, GWAS have been extremely successful for many complex disorders. They have pointed to numerous candidate loci for type 1 and type 2 diabetes, Crohn's disease, coronary artery disease, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, multiple sclerosis and many other diseases. Some of these loci were previously implicated as sites of known rare mutations that cause obviously Mendelian forms of the disease in question, whereas others are novel findings that implicate new genes in disease processes. For some disorders, the findings converge on particular biochemical processes or pathways, such as beta-cell dysfunction or insulin action in type 2 diabetes [28, 29]; natriuretic peptide signaling in high blood pressure and cardiovascular disease risk [30]; immune system genes in multiple sclerosis [31]; innate immunity and inflammation in Crohn's disease [32]; and neural development in schizophrenia [33, 34]. These studies have also revealed some shared genetic risk across multiple disorders, including various autoimmune disorders (type 1 diabetes, Crohn's disease, multiple sclerosis and others) [35] and between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder [33, 34, 36].

The general trend across these studies is that the SNPs that give statistically significant association signals have tiny effects on disease risk, with odds ratios typically in the region of 1.05 to 1.2 (which means that if you carry such an allele, your risk of disease is increased 1.05- to 1.2-fold). This is exactly as predicted under a polygenic model: individual variants are not expected to have large individual effects. The aggregate risk caused by all the identified variants considered together is, however, also still relatively small. In most cases, the SNPs that meet the criteria for genome-wide significance can collectively mathematically explain only a small percentage of the genetic variance of the disorder [37] and hardly any of the familial risk [38]. There is also no statistical evidence for the kind of epistatic interactions that might be expected: combinations of alleles simply increase the overall effect additively (for example, for height [39, 40] and body-mass index [41]).

There are several, not mutually exclusive ways to interpret the positive signals that have emerged from GWAS of complex disorders (Figure 2). First, they represent the actual effects of either the genotyped SNP or a common variant that is in LD with it. Given the power of the studies and the lack of overall variance explained by the identified variants, one could further conclude that, while real, the overall effect of such common variants is very modest [42, 43]. This would be consistent with a model in which such variants act as modifiers of rare mutations, but, even in aggregate, do not cause disease in the absence of some such mutation.

Figure 2
figure 2

Interpreting positive GWAS signals. (a) GWAS can point to chromosomal loci that may harbor causal variants. (b) The associated SNP will act as a marker of multiple additional common variants in an LD block. The marker SNP or any of these other common variants could be the causal variant. The very low odds ratio across the population might represent a tiny effect of one of these variants in every individual or a large effect that arises only in the context of some rare mutation. (c) Alternatively, at least some common SNP signals could actually be tagging rare variants of large effect in the population, which are in strong LD with it (stars). If these occur, by chance, more prevalently on one haplotype than on another, this will lead to a slightly increased frequency of one allele in cases when compared to controls (that is, an association signal).

Second, the common variants found represent merely the tip of the iceberg. Many other variants exist that have even smaller effect sizes, which current studies are underpowered to detect. Collectively, these could explain a sizeable fraction of the overall genetic variance - much more than actually observed, due to incomplete LD with the causal variants - leaving little need to invoke rare mutations (for example, [33]). Simulations exploring this possibility are discussed in Box 2.

Third, the common SNPs that show association signals are actually tagging rare mutations that segregate in the sampled populations [23, 27]. Whenever a rare mutation arises, it necessarily occurs on the background of some ancient haplotype. If such a mutation predisposes to disease with relatively high penetrance, explaining even a small percentage of cases in a population, then the common haplotype will be slightly increased in frequency among cases when compared to controls. The odds ratio of the associated common SNP, which suggests a very modest increase in risk, could thus actually signal a highly penetrant variant on a fraction of the chromosomes with that haplotype. This kind of effect will be especially prevalent in studies from small, defined populations. Though one might expect it to be diluted out when multiple populations are combined (because different rare mutations in the same gene will occur on different haplotypes), it has been argued that synthetic associations with a single SNP allele can arise by chance due to multiple rare variants in the same locus [23, 27], though others contend this is unlikely [21, 22].

It is not possible to determine definitively which of these interpretations is correct from the GWAS data themselves. In particular, the strongly worded claim that GWAS signals provide strong support for a polygenic architecture of complex disorders, involving large numbers of common variants of small effect in each individual [33], is not justified [18, 27, 44] (Box 2). GWAS simply cannot determine whether the alleles responsible for the positive associations are common or rare, nor can aggregate scores or genome-partitioning models [45, 46] tell how many alleles are involved, either across the population or in each affected individual (especially if the assumption that the signals from the SNPs in question are independent is not valid (D Goldstein, personal communication)).

Fortunately, we do not have to rely on statistical simulations to answer these questions. There is direct empirical evidence that rare mutations play the predominant role in the inheritance of such disorders. This evidence includes the nature of the spectrum of genetic variation in humans and the growing number of examples of identified, rare, disease-causing mutations.

The spectrum of human genetic variation

There is a common view that the human genome can be divided into bases that are pretty much constant across all people and those that are polymorphic. The logical extension of this idea is that heritable phenotypic differences between people must be caused by the particular combinations of polymorphisms that they inherit at the variable sites (for example, [47]). Recent data from whole-genome sequencing efforts show just how wrong this view is.

Far from most of the genome being effectively constant, it seems that every position in the genome has been mutated many, many times over in the human population [48–51]. Each of us carries thousands of very rare variants, including hundreds of novel mutations [52–56]. Recent, rare mutations are far more likely to have a deleterious effect on protein production or function and much more likely to cause disease than common variants [54–59].

New mutations may spread in the pedigree or population in which they arise for some time, depending largely on whether they have a deleterious effect on fitness or not [51, 60, 61]. Mutations that do have a deleterious effect will be quickly selected against, though the recent human population explosion could allow less penetrant or recessive alleles to persist for some time at low frequency [50, 60]. It is, however, highly paradoxical to suppose that variants that predispose to serious diseases would ever rise to a high frequency [51, 62, 63]. The casual invocation of balancing selection as a mechanism to maintain disease-causing alleles at high frequency is not supported by any evidence [64]. If a disorder is associated with reduced fitness, then the distribution of alleles affecting it will be expected to shift towards very rare ones that are highly deleterious [65] (and not, as previously concluded for disorders for which it was assumed that no correlation with fitness exists, towards slightly deleterious alleles at moderate frequency [66]).

Identifying rare mutations

The best evidence that so-called common disorders really encompass many distinct genetic disorders is the growing numbers of rare, highly penetrant mutations causing such disorders that are now being identified. Examples of single mutations causing disorders such as autism, schizophrenia, diabetes, epilepsy and many other common diseases have long been known. While these could be identified in only a small proportion of cases, they could, however, be disregarded as exceptions to the generality of the disease (for example, [67]): they did not cause 'real schizophrenia' or 'real autism'. But what if there is no such thing? What if all cases are due to some rare mutation? The growing number of cases explained by such examples makes this view more and more difficult to argue against.

Such cases include copy number variants (CNVs; that is, deletions or duplications of sections of chromosomes, which often affect more than one gene), as well as point mutations. CNVs have become more easily detected, using genomic microarray and sequencing technologies, and have been found to contribute significantly to the total number of cases of a range of psychiatric and neurological disorders, including schizophrenia [68–76], autism [73, 77–80], attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder [81–84], Tourette syndrome [85], developmental delay and mental retardation [14], and epilepsy [86].

Whole-genome or whole-exome sequencing strategies are now also identifying many point mutations that predispose with high penetrance to various disorders. Studies on psychiatric disorders have again led the way here [13, 87–93], but recent reports have also identified single mutations causing neonatal diabetes mellitus [94], coronary artery disease [95] and Crohn's disease [96].

Real sources of complexity in linking genotype to phenotype

If complex disorders really arise due to rare mutations, then why is their inheritance not more obviously Mendelian? There are a number of factors that contribute to the complexity of inheritance of these disorders. I have argued that much of the complexity is simply apparent, due to lumping together what are actually distinct disorders under one umbrella term. Certain pathophysiological states could arise due to mutation in any of a large number of different genes. There are clearly many ways to cause psychosis or seizures or poor control of blood sugar level, just as there are many different ways, genetically, to cause blindness or deafness or mental retardation. More common diseases are likely to reflect a larger mutational target (that is, more genes will be involved in the affected underlying processes) [97].

This can only be part of the answer, however. Even within single families, the inheritance patterns of these phenotypes are usually not simple. What other factors might contribute to this complexity? First, the definition of the phenotype is probably very imprecise. A major finding that has emerged from recent studies is that specific mutations do not respect the boundaries of diagnostic categories - their effects can manifest in many different ways, leading to different symptoms and diagnoses in different carriers [15, 17, 98–103]. Analyzing segregation patterns on the basis of overly specific diagnostic categories could thus be highly misleading.

Second, any particular mutation could be required but not sufficient to cause disease in individual carriers [17]. It is extremely common, the norm actually, for Mendelian mutations to be modified by additional variants in the genetic background [104, 105]. Some highly penetrant mutations will be clearly responsible for disease, by themselves. For diseases that have a strong effect on fitness, these will be almost immediately selected against and therefore enriched in sporadic cases that are caused by a novel occurrence of the mutation. Mutations that have lower penetrance might require additional 'hits' to result in the disease phenotype. There are numerous examples of compound heterozygosity or digenic inheritance underlying complex disorders [51, 106–110]. Oligogenic interactions, involving several mutations, might also be important in some cases. Epistatic interactions between multiple mutations could be highly complex and unpredictable [111–114], or even paradoxical, as in cases where two disease mutations suppress each other's effects [115, 116].

Finally, non-genetic factors must also be important in the emergence of disease phenotypes, given the incomplete concordance in the phenotypes of monozygotic twins for many of these diseases. Incomplete penetrance and variable expressivity could result from environmental factors and also from intrinsic developmental variation. The latter is especially important for neurodevelopmental disorders, where a certain probability that a pathogenic route will be followed could be inherited but the actual outcome of development would be strongly influenced by stochastic events [117].

Concluding remarks

The apparent complexity of common disorders arises to a large extent because of our poor ability to discriminate between what are in reality many distinct genetic disorders. Most cases of such disorders are likely to be the result of a rare, recent mutation that has a strong biological effect, or of interactions between a small number of such mutations. GWAS point to loci that might be involved in complex disorders but the population-based metrics that they provide say little about the number or type of causal alleles in individuals. Modifying effects of common alleles are certainly possible, though the evidence for these remains indirect. This change in paradigm is already having an impact in the clinic, as more and more cases of complex disorders are clinically defined on the basis of a genetic diagnosis, indexing the primary cause of the disease and not merely the surface symptoms [94–96, 118, 119].

Box 1. Falconer's fantasy

The multifactorial threshold model has some intuitive appeal, especially as an expression of an interaction between some genetic predisposition or vulnerability and the effects of an environmental stressor. This is not, however, how it is used in the context of the genetic architecture of complex disorders. Here, it is purely the genetic components of variance that are being modeled (Figure 1).

Visscher and colleagues [67, 120] have, rather surprisingly, used height as an example to illustrate the liability threshold model. Height is clearly continuously distributed in the population. They nevertheless imagine a disease called 'loftiness', which afflicts those above some arbitrary height threshold. In this scenario, even families that 'we consider tall might not have many individuals passing the threshold into loftiness', supposedly paralleling the situation in a complex disorder like schizophrenia. As it happens, height is a perfect example to illustrate why the threshold model makes no biological sense. There is no such threshold. An increasing burden of height risk alleles does not push people into gigantism - single mutations do (for example, [121]; and the same is true for dwarfism at the other end of the spectrum [122, 123]). In fact, the aggregate effects of the multiple common variants that affect height are remarkably linear [39, 40].

Complex systems are typically robust to the cumulative effects of small variations; in fact, they must be so in order to withstand the inherent noise in biochemical systems and effects from variables outside the system [124, 125]. The supposed tiny effects on expression level of common variants are highly unlikely to have a large effect precisely because the system has such fluctuations on a moment-to-moment basis as a constant (and essential) feature [126, 127]. In particular, the small-world architecture of complex networks is robust to many small changes but paradoxically vulnerable to 'attack' of certain nodes [128]; these networks can fail catastrophically in response to large changes in specific components.

Box 2. Aggregate polygenic scores

GWAS for various disorders have identified a number of SNPs that are above the threshold for genome-wide statistical significance as disease risk factors. Collectively, these loci explain a small fraction of risk. It remains possible, however, that additional SNPs are also associated with risk but that their signals are buried in the noise and cannot meet the burden of multiple testing correction with current sample sizes. To attempt to measure whether additional associated SNPs exist, various researchers have generated aggregate scores for the top x percentage of SNPs based on a ranking of P-values in a discovery sample. The scores for each individual in a replication sample are then used to see if they can distinguish cases and controls. In a study on schizophrenia, such an aggregate score, involving thousands of SNPs, was indeed significantly increased in cases versus controls. However, this score accounted for only 3% of the variance [33]. This effect has since been replicated in a family study, where it explained 5% of the variance [129], ruling out population stratification concerns, and in a much larger GWAS, where it explained 6% of the variance [34].

These data indicate that there probably are additional SNPs that are associated with schizophrenia risk that could be detected with larger samples. Indeed, the most recent GWAS mega-analysis for schizophrenia [34] reports additional significant SNPs that were not found in earlier studies. Taken at face value, however, they also suggest that the overall contribution of common variants to the genetic variance that affects the disorder is very modest (less than 10%). But based on the idea that the real signals might be swamped out by non-associated SNPs in these aggregate scores, and that the linkage between the genotyped and the putative causal SNPs was probably imperfect, the authors performed simulations where they suggest that the overall impact of common SNPs was in fact much higher (as much as 33% of the total variance). Despite the claims of convergence onto a narrow range of values, seven models (out of 560 tested), involving combinations of multiple parameters such as allele frequency, effect size, number of SNPs involved and others, actually give wildly different estimates of the total true variance explained (from 34% to 98%) and the number of SNPs contributing (from 6% to 100%). The actual lack of convergence does not provoke much confidence in the overall claim that these results 'provide molecular genetic evidence for a substantial polygenic component to the risk of schizophrenia involving thousands of common alleles of very small effect.'

That claim assumes (circularly) that the associations of common SNPs actually reflect the biological effects of common variants, as opposed to associations due to tagged rare variants [23, 27]. It also assumes that the signals carried by the SNPs used in the score are independent: if not, then no information can be deduced as to the actual number of underlying causal variants (D Goldstein, personal communication). For now, the conclusion that many loci are involved in the genetics of schizophrenia across the population is uncontested. The conclusion that thousands of loci are causally involved in the inheritance of the disorder in each individual is not justified.

Abbreviations

CNV:

copy number variant

GWAS:

genome-wide association study

LD:

linkage disequilibrium

RP:

retinitis pigmentosa

SNP:

single nucleotide polymorphism.

References

  1. Plomin R, Haworth CM, Davis OS: Common disorders are quantitative traits. Nat Rev Genet. 2009, 10: 872-878. 10.1038/ni.1747.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Gottesman II, Shields J: A polygenic theory of schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1967, 58: 199-205. 10.1073/pnas.58.1.199.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. McGue M, Gottesman II, Rao DC: Resolving genetic models for the transmission of schizophrenia. Genet Epidemiol. 1985, 2: 99-110. 10.1002/gepi.1370020110.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Risch N: Genetic linkage and complex diseases, with special reference to psychiatric disorders. Genet Epidemiol. 1990, 7: 3-16. 10.1002/gepi.1370070103. discussion 17-45

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. O'Rourke DH, Gottesman II, Suarez BK, Rice J, Reich T: Refutation of the general single-locus model for the etiology of schizophrenia. Am J Hum Genet. 1982, 34: 630-649.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Risch N: Linkage strategies for genetically complex traits. I. Multilocus models. Am J Hum Genet. 1990, 46: 222-228.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. International HapMap Consortium: The International HapMap Project. Nature. 2003, 426: 789-796. 10.1038/nature02168.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Risch N, Merikangas K: The future of genetic studies of complex human diseases. Science. 1996, 273: 1516-1517. 10.1126/science.273.5281.1516.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Reich DE, Lander ES: On the allelic spectrum of human disease. Trends Genet. 2001, 17: 502-510. 10.1016/S0168-9525(01)02410-6.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Hirschhorn JN, Daly MJ: Genome-wide association studies for common diseases and complex traits. Nat Rev Genet. 2005, 6: 95-108.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Wright AF, Hastie ND: Complex genetic diseases: controversy over the Croesus code. Genome Biol. 2001, 2: COMMENT2007

    Google Scholar 

  12. Wright AF, Chakarova CF, Abd El-Aziz MM, Bhattacharya SS: Photoreceptor degeneration: genetic and mechanistic dissection of a complex trait. Nat Rev Genet. 2010, 11: 273-284.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Najmabadi H, Hu H, Garshasbi M, Zemojtel T, Abedini SS, Chen W, Hosseini M, Behjati F, Haas S, Jamali P, Zecha A, Mohseni M, Püttmann L, Vahid LN, Jensen C, Moheb LA, Bienek M, Larti F, Mueller I, Weissmann R, Darvish H, Wrogemann K, Hadavi V, Lipkowitz B, Esmaeeli-Nieh S, Wieczorek D, Kariminejad R, Firouzabadi SG, Cohen M, Fattahi Z, et al: Deep sequencing reveals 50 novel genes for recessive cognitive disorders. Nature. 2011, 478: 57-63. 10.1038/nature10423.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Cooper GM, Coe BP, Girirajan S, Rosenfeld JA, Vu TH, Baker C, Williams C, Stalker H, Hamid R, Hannig V, Abdel-Hamid H, Bader P, McCracken E, Niyazov D, Leppig K, Thiese H, Hummel M, Alexander N, Gorski J, Kussmann J, Shashi V, Johnson K, Rehder C, Ballif BC, Shaffer LG, Eichler EE: A copy number variation morbidity map of developmental delay. Nat Genet. 2011, 43: 838-846. 10.1038/ng.909.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Craddock N, Owen MJ: The Kraepelinian dichotomy - going, going ... but still not gone. Br J Psychiatry. 2010, 196: 92-95. 10.1192/bjp.bp.109.073429.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. McClellan JM, Susser E, King MC: Schizophrenia: a common disease caused by multiple rare alleles. Br J Psychiatry. 2007, 190: 194-199. 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.025585.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Mitchell KJ: The genetics of neurodevelopmental disease. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2011, 21: 197-203. 10.1016/j.conb.2010.08.009.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Mitchell KJ, Porteous DJ: Rethinking the genetic architecture of schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2011, 41: 19-32. 10.1017/S003329171000070X.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Betancur C: Etiological heterogeneity in autism spectrum disorders: more than 100 genetic and genomic disorders and still counting. Brain Res. 2011, 1380: 42-77.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. James JW: Frequency in relatives for an all-or-none trait. Ann Hum Genet. 1971, 35: 47-49. 10.1111/j.1469-1809.1956.tb01377.x.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Anderson CA, Soranzo N, Zeggini E, Barrett JC: Synthetic associations are unlikely to account for many common disease genome-wide association signals. PLoS Biol. 2011, 9: e1000580-10.1371/journal.pbio.1000580.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Wray NR, Purcell SM, Visscher PM: Synthetic associations created by rare variants do not explain most GWAS results. PLoS Biol. 2011, 9: e1000579-10.1371/journal.pbio.1000579.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Goldstein DB: The importance of synthetic associations will only be resolved empirically. PLoS Biol. 2011, 9: e1001008-10.1371/journal.pbio.1001008.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Dempster ER, Lerner IM: Heritability of threshold characters. Genetics. 1950, 35: 212-236.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Falconer DS: Introduction to Quantitative Genetics. 1989, New York: Longman Scientific and Technical, 3

    Google Scholar 

  26. Fisher RA: The correlation between relatives on the supposition of Mendelian inheritance. Trans R Soc Edinb. 1918, 52: 399-433.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Dickson SP, Wang K, Krantz I, Hakonarson H, Goldstein DB: Rare variants create synthetic genome-wide associations. PLoS Biol. 2010, 8: e1000294-10.1371/journal.pbio.1000294.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Voight BF, Scott LJ, Steinthorsdottir V, Morris AP, Dina C, Welch RP, Zeggini E, Huth C, Aulchenko YS, Thorleifsson G, McCulloch LJ, Ferreira T, Grallert H, Amin N, Wu G, Willer CJ, Raychaudhuri S, McCarroll SA, Langenberg C, Hofmann OM, Dupuis J, Qi L, Segrè AV, van Hoek M, Navarro P, Ardlie K, Balkau B, Benediktsson R, Bennett AJ, Blagieva R, et al: Twelve type 2 diabetes susceptibility loci identified through large-scale association analysis. Nat Genet. 2010, 42: 579-589. 10.1038/ng.609.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Imamura M, Maeda S: Genetics of type 2 diabetes: the GWAS era and future perspectives. Endocr J. 2011, 58: 723-739. 10.1507/endocrj.EJ11-0113.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. International Consortium for Blood Pressure Genome-Wide Association Studies, Ehret GB, Munroe PB, Rice KM, Bochud M, Johnson AD, Chasman DI, Smith AV, Tobin MD, Verwoert GC, Hwang SJ, Pihur V, Vollenweider P, O'Reilly PF, Amin N, Bragg-Gresham JL, Teumer A, Glazer NL, Launer L, Zhao JH, Aulchenko Y, Heath S, Sõber S, Parsa A, Luan J, Arora P, Dehghan A, Zhang F, Lucas G, Hicks AA, et al: Genetic variants in novel pathways influence blood pressure and cardiovascular disease risk. Nature. 2011, 478: 103-109. 10.1038/nature10405.

    Google Scholar 

  31. International Multiple Sclerosis Genetics Consortium; Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium 2, Sawcer S, Hellenthal G, Pirinen M, Spencer CC, Patsopoulos NA, Moutsianas L, Dilthey A, Su Z, Freeman C, Hunt SE, Edkins S, Gray E, Booth DR, Potter SC, Goris A, Band G, Oturai AB, Strange A, Saarela J, Bellenguez C, Fontaine B, Gillman M, Hemmer B, Gwilliam R, Zipp F, Jayakumar A, Martin R, Leslie S, et al: Genetic risk and a primary role for cell-mediated immune mechanisms in multiple sclerosis. Nature. 2011, 476: 214-219. 10.1038/nature10251.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Mathew CG: New links to the pathogenesis of Crohn disease provided by genome-wide association scans. Nat Rev Genet. 2008, 9: 9-14.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Purcell SM, Wray NR, Stone JL, Visscher PM, O'Donovan MC, Sullivan PF, Sklar P: Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature. 2009, 460: 748-752.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Ripke S, Sanders AR, Kendler KS, Levinson DF, Sklar P, Holmans PA, Lin DY, Duan J, Ophoff RA, Andreassen OA, Scolnick E, Cichon S, St Clair D, Corvin A, Gurling H, Werge T, Rujescu D, Blackwood DH, Pato CN, Malhotra AK, Purcell S, Dudbridge F, Neale BM, Rossin L, Visscher PM, Posthuma D, Ruderfer DM, Fanous A, Stefansson H, Steinberg S, et al: Genome-wide association study identifies five new schizophrenia loci. Nat Genet. 2011, 43: 969-976. 10.1038/ng.940.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Cotsapas C, Voight BF, Rossin E, Lage K, Neale BM, Wallace C, Abecasis GR, Barrett JC, Behrens T, Cho J, De Jager PL, Elder JT, Graham RR, Gregersen P, Klareskog L, Siminovitch KA, van Heel DA, Wijmenga C, Worthington J, Todd JA, Hafler DA, Rich SS, Daly MJ, FOCiS Network of Consortia: Pervasive sharing of genetic effects in autoimmune disease. PLoS Genet. 2011, 7: e1002254-10.1371/journal.pgen.1002254.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Sklar P, Ripke S, Scott LJ, Andreassen OA, Cichon S, Craddock N, Edenberg HJ, Nurnberger JI, Rietschel M, Blackwood D, Corvin A, Flickinger M, Guan W, Mattingsdal M, McQuillin A, Kwan P, Wienker TF, Daly M, Dudbridge F, Holmans PA, Lin D, Burmeister M, Greenwood TA, Hamshere ML, Muglia P, Smith EN, Zandi PP, Nievergelt CM, McKinney R, Shilling PD, et al: Large-scale genome-wide association analysis of bipolar disorder identifies a new susceptibility locus near ODZ4. Nat Genet. 2011, 43: 977-983. 10.1038/ng.943.

    CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Manolio TA, Collins FS, Cox NJ, Goldstein DB, Hindorff LA, Hunter DJ, McCarthy MI, Ramos EM, Cardon LR, Chakravarti A, Cho JH, Guttmacher AE, Kong A, Kruglyak L, Mardis E, Rotimi CN, Slatkin M, Valle D, Whittemore AS, Boehnke M, Clark AG, Eichler EE, Gibson G, Haines JL, Mackay TF, McCarroll SA, Visscher PM: Finding the missing heritability of complex diseases. Nature. 2009, 461: 747-753. 10.1038/nature08494.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Hemminki K, Forsti A, Bermejo JL: The 'common disease-common variant' hypothesis and familial risks. PLoS ONE. 2008, 3: e2504-10.1371/journal.pone.0002504.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Weedon MN, Lango H, Lindgren CM, Wallace C, Evans DM, Mangino M, Freathy RM, Perry JR, Stevens S, Hall AS, Samani NJ, Shields B, Prokopenko I, Farrall M, Dominiczak A, Diabetes Genetics Initiative, Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium, Johnson T, Bergmann S, Beckmann JS, Vollenweider P, Waterworth DM, Mooser V, Palmer CN, Morris AD, Ouwehand WH, Cambridge GEM Consortium, Zhao JH, Li S, Loos RJ, et al: Genome-wide association analysis identifies 20 loci that influence adult height. Nat Genet. 2008, 40: 575-583. 10.1038/ng.121.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Okada Y, Kamatani Y, Takahashi A, Matsuda K, Hosono N, Ohmiya H, Daigo Y, Yamamoto K, Kubo M, Nakamura Y, Kamatani N: A genome-wide association study in 19633 Japanese subjects identified LHX3-QSOX2 and IGF1 as adult height loci. Hum Mol Genet. 2010, 19: 2303-2312. 10.1093/hmg/ddq091.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Speliotes EK, Willer CJ, Berndt SI, Monda KL, Thorleifsson G, Jackson AU, Allen HL, Lindgren CM, Luan J, Mägi R, Randall JC, Vedantam S, Winkler TW, Qi L, Workalemahu T, Heid IM, Steinthorsdottir V, Stringham HM, Weedon MN, Wheeler E, Wood AR, Ferreira T, Weyant RJ, Segrè AV, Estrada K, Liang L, Nemesh J, Park JH, Gustafsson S, Kilpeläinen TO, et al: Association analyses of 249,796 individuals reveal 18 new loci associated with body mass index. Nat Genet. 2010, 42: 937-948. 10.1038/ng.686.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Need AC, Ge D, Weale ME, Maia J, Feng S, Heinzen EL, Shianna KV, Yoon W, Kasperaviciūte D, Gennarelli M, Strittmatter WJ, Bonvicini C, Rossi G, Jayathilake K, Cola PA, McEvoy JP, Keefe RS, Fisher EM, St Jean PL, Giegling I, Hartmann AM, Möller HJ, Ruppert A, Fraser G, Crombie C, Middleton LT, St Clair D, Roses AD, Muglia P, Francks C, et al: A genome-wide investigation of SNPs and CNVs in schizophrenia. PLoS Genet. 2009, 5: e1000373-10.1371/journal.pgen.1000373.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Kasperaviciūte D, Catarino CB, Heinzen EL, Depondt C, Cavalleri GL, Caboclo LO, Tate SK, Jamnadas-Khoda J, Chinthapalli K, Clayton LM, Shianna KV, Radtke RA, Mikati MA, Gallentine WB, Husain AM, Alhusaini S, Leppert D, Middleton LT, Gibson RA, Johnson MR, Matthews PM, Hosford D, Heuser K, Amos L, Ortega M, Zumsteg D, Wieser HG, Steinhoff BJ, Krämer G, Hansen J, et al: Common genetic variation and susceptibility to partial epilepsies: a genome-wide association study. Brain. 2010, 133: 2136-2147. 10.1093/brain/awq130.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. McClellan J, King MC: Genomic analysis of mental illness: a changing landscape. JAMA. 2010, 303: 2523-2524. 10.1001/jama.2010.869.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Yang J, Benyamin B, McEvoy BP, Gordon S, Henders AK, Nyholt DR, Madden PA, Heath AC, Martin NG, Montgomery GW, Goddard ME, Visscher PM: Common SNPs explain a large proportion of the heritability for human height. Nat Genet. 2010, 42: 565-569. 10.1038/ng.608.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Yang J, Manolio TA, Pasquale LR, Boerwinkle E, Caporaso N, Cunningham JM, de Andrade M, Feenstra B, Feingold E, Hayes MG, Hill WG, Landi MT, Alonso A, Lettre G, Lin P, Ling H, Lowe W, Mathias RA, Melbye M, Pugh E, Cornelis MC, Weir BS, Goddard ME, Visscher PM: Genome partitioning of genetic variation for complex traits using common SNPs. Nat Genet. 2011, 43: 519-525. 10.1038/ng.823.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Roberts R, Wells GA, Stewart AF, Dandona S, Chen L: The genome-wide association study - a new era for common polygenic disorders. J Cardiovasc Transl Res. 2010, 3: 173-182. 10.1007/s12265-010-9178-6.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Walsh CA, Engle EC: Allelic diversity in human developmental neurogenetics: insights into biology and disease. Neuron. 2010, 68: 245-253. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.042.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. McClellan J, King MC: Genetic heterogeneity in human disease. Cell. 2010, 141: 210-217. 10.1016/j.cell.2010.03.032.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Coventry A, Bull-Otterson LM, Liu X, Clark AG, Maxwell TJ, Crosby J, Hixson JE, Rea TJ, Muzny DM, Lewis LR, Wheeler DA, Sabo A, Lusk C, Weiss KG, Akbar H, Cree A, Hawes AC, Newsham I, Varghese RT, Villasana D, Gross S, Joshi V, Santibanez J, Morgan M, Chang K, Iv WH, Templeton AR, Boerwinkle E, Gibbs R, Sing CF: Deep resequencing reveals excess rare recent variants consistent with explosive population growth. Nat Commun. 2010, 1: 131-10.1038/ncomms1130.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Lupski JR, Belmont JW, Boerwinkle E, Gibbs RA: Clan genomics and the complex architecture of human disease. Cell. 2011, 147: 32-43. 10.1016/j.cell.2011.09.008.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Coventry A, Bull-Otterson LM, Liu X, Clark AG, Maxwell TJ, Crosby J, Hixson JE, Rea TJ, Muzny DM, Lewis LR, Wheeler DA, Sabo A, Lusk C, Weiss KG, Akbar H, Cree A, Hawes AC, Newsham I, Varghese RT, Villasana D, Gross S, Joshi V, Santibanez J, Morgan M, Chang K, Iv WH, Templeton AR, Boerwinkle E, Gibbs R, Sing CF: Targeted capture and massively parallel sequencing of 12 human exomes. Nature. 2009, 461: 272-276. 10.1038/nature08250.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Pelak K, Shianna KV, Ge D, Maia JM, Zhu M, Smith JP, Cirulli ET, Fellay J, Dickson SP, Gumbs CE, Heinzen EL, Need AC, Ruzzo EK, Singh A, Campbell CR, Hong LK, Lornsen KA, McKenzie AM, Sobreira NL, Hoover-Fong JE, Milner JD, Ottman R, Haynes BF, Goedert JJ, Goldstein DB: The characterization of twenty sequenced human genomes. PLoS Genet. 2010, 6: e1001111-10.1371/journal.pgen.1001111.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Durbin RM, Abecasis GR, Altshuler DL, Auton A, Brooks LD, Gibbs RA, Hurles ME, McVean GA: A map of human genome variation from population-scale sequencing. Nature. 2010, 467: 1061-1073. 10.1038/nature09534.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  55. Li Y, Vinckenbosch N, Tian G, Huerta-Sanchez E, Jiang T, Jiang H, Albrechtsen A, Andersen G, Cao H, Korneliussen T, Grarup N, Guo Y, Hellman I, Jin X, Li Q, Liu J, Liu X, Sparsø T, Tang M, Wu H, Wu R, Yu C, Zheng H, Astrup A, Bolund L, Holmkvist J, Jørgensen T, Kristiansen K, Schmitz O, Schwartz TW, et al: Resequencing of 200 human exomes identifies an excess of low-frequency non-synonymous coding variants. Nat Genet. 2010, 42: 969-972. 10.1038/ng.680.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  56. Marth GT, Yu F, Indap AR, Garimella K, Gravel S, Leong WF, Tyler-Smith C, Bainbridge M, Blackwell T, Zheng-Bradley X, Chen Y, Challis D, Clarke L, Ball EV, Cibulskis K, Cooper DN, Fulton B, Hartl C, Koboldt D, Muzny D, Smith R, Sougnez C, Stewart C, Ward A, Yu J, Xue Y, Altshuler D, Bustamante CD, Clark AG, Daly M, et al: The functional spectrum of low-frequency coding variation. Genome Biol. 2011, 12: R84-10.1186/gb-2011-12-9-r84.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Kryukov GV, Pennacchio LA, Sunyaev SR: Most rare missense alleles are deleterious in humans: implications for complex disease and association studies. Am J Hum Genet. 2007, 80: 727-739. 10.1086/513473.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Zhu Q, Ge D, Maia JM, Zhu M, Petrovski S, Dickson SP, Heinzen EL, Shianna KV, Goldstein DB: A genome-wide comparison of the functional properties of rare and common genetic variants in humans. Am J Hum Genet. 2011, 88: 458-468. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2011.03.008.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Gorlov IP, Gorlova OY, Frazier ML, Spitz MR, Amos CI: Evolutionary evidence of the effect of rare variants on disease etiology. Clin Genet. 2011, 79: 199-206. 10.1111/j.1399-0004.2010.01535.x.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Boyko AR, Williamson SH, Indap AR, Degenhardt JD, Hernandez RD, Lohmueller KE, Adams MD, Schmidt S, Sninsky JJ, Sunyaev SR, White TJ, Nielsen R, Clark AG, Bustamante CD: Assessing the evolutionary impact of amino acid mutations in the human genome. PLoS Genet. 2008, 4: e1000083-10.1371/journal.pgen.1000083.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Gravel S, Henn BM, Gutenkunst RN, Indap AR, Marth GT, Clark AG, Yu F, Gibbs RA, Bustamante CD: Demographic history and rare allele sharing among human populations. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011, 108: 11983-11988. 10.1073/pnas.1019276108.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Keller MC, Miller G: Resolving the paradox of common, harmful, heritable mental disorders: which evolutionary genetic models work best?. Behav Brain Sci. 2006, 29: 385-404. discussion 405-352

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Uher R: The role of genetic variation in the causation of mental illness: an evolution-informed framework. Mol Psychiatry. 2009, 14: 1072-1082. 10.1038/mp.2009.85.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  64. Mayo O: The rise and fall of the common disease-common variant (CD-CV) hypothesis: how the sickle cell disease paradigm led us all astray (or did it?). Twin Res Hum Genet. 2007, 10: 793-804. 10.1375/twin.10.6.793.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Eyre-Walker A: Evolution in health and medicine Sackler colloquium: genetic architecture of a complex trait and its implications for fitness and genome-wide association studies. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010, 107 (Suppl 1): 1752-1756.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Pritchard JK: Are rare variants responsible for susceptibility to complex diseases?. Am J Hum Genet. 2001, 69: 124-137. 10.1086/321272.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Wray NR, Visscher PM: Narrowing the boundaries of the genetic architecture of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2010, 36: 14-23. 10.1093/schbul/sbp137.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Walsh T, McClellan JM, McCarthy SE, Addington AM, Pierce SB, Cooper GM, Nord AS, Kusenda M, Malhotra D, Bhandari A, Stray SM, Rippey CF, Roccanova P, Makarov V, Lakshmi B, Findling RL, Sikich L, Stromberg T, Merriman B, Gogtay N, Butler P, Eckstrand K, Noory L, Gochman P, Long R, Chen Z, Davis S, Baker C, Eichler EE, Meltzer PS, et al: Rare structural variants disrupt multiple genes in neurodevelopmental pathways in schizophrenia. Science. 2008, 320: 539-543. 10.1126/science.1155174.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  69. Xu B, Roos JL, Levy S, van Rensburg EJ, Gogos JA, Karayiorgou M: Strong association of de novo copy number mutations with sporadic schizophrenia. Nat Genet. 2008, 40: 880-885. 10.1038/ng.162.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  70. Stefansson H, Rujescu D, Cichon S, Pietiläinen OP, Ingason A, Steinberg S, Fossdal R, Sigurdsson E, Sigmundsson T, Buizer-Voskamp JE, Hansen T, Jakobsen KD, Muglia P, Francks C, Matthews PM, Gylfason A, Halldorsson BV, Gudbjartsson D, Thorgeirsson TE, Sigurdsson A, Jonasdottir A, Jonasdottir A, Bjornsson A, Mattiasdottir S, Blondal T, Haraldsson M, Magnusdottir BB, Giegling I, Möller HJ, Hartmann A, et al: Large recurrent microdeletions associated with schizophrenia. Nature. 2008, 455: 232-236. 10.1038/nature07229.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. International Schizophrenia Consortium: Rare chromosomal deletions and duplications increase risk of schizophrenia. Nature. 2008, 455: 237-241. 10.1038/nature07239.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Shi YY, He G, Zhang Z, Tang W, Zhang J, Zhao Q, Zhang J, Li XW, Xi ZR, Fang C, Zhao XZ, Feng GY, He L: A study of rare structural variants in schizophrenia patients and normal controls from Chinese Han population. Mol Psychiatry. 2008, 13: 911-913. 10.1038/mp.2008.69.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  73. Awadalla P, Gauthier J, Myers RA, Casals F, Hamdan FF, Griffing AR, Côté M, Henrion E, Spiegelman D, Tarabeux J, Piton A, Yang Y, Boyko A, Bustamante C, Xiong L, Rapoport JL, Addington AM, DeLisi JL, Krebs MO, Joober R, Millet B, Fombonne E, Mottron L, Zilversmit M, Keebler J, Daoud H, Marineau C, Roy-Gagnon MH, Dubé MP, Eyre-Walker A, et al: Direct measure of the de novo mutation rate in autism and schizophrenia cohorts. Am J Hum Genet. 2010, 87: 316-324. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.07.019.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Pinto D, Pagnamenta AT, Klei L, Anney R, Merico D, Regan R, Conroy J, Magalhaes TR, Correia C, Abrahams BS, Almeida J, Bacchelli E, Bader GD, Bailey AJ, Baird G, Battaglia A, Berney T, Bolshakova N, Bölte S, Bolton PF, Bourgeron T, Brennan S, Brian J, Bryson SE, Carson AR, Casallo G, Casey J, Chung BH, Cochrane L, Corsello C, et al: Functional impact of global rare copy number variation in autism spectrum disorders. Nature. 2010, 466: 368-372. 10.1038/nature09146.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Levinson DF, Duan J, Oh S, Wang K, Sanders AR, Shi J, Zhang N, Mowry BJ, Olincy A, Amin F, Cloninger CR, Silverman JM, Buccola NG, Byerley WF, Black DW, Kendler KS, Freedman R, Dudbridge F, Pe'er I, Hakonarson H, Bergen SE, Fanous AH, Holmans PA, Gejman PV: Copy number variants in schizophrenia: confirmation of five previous findings and new evidence for 3q29 microdeletions and VIPR2 duplications. Am J Psychiatry. 2011, 168: 302-316. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10060876.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Vacic V, McCarthy S, Malhotra D, Murray F, Chou HH, Peoples A, Makarov V, Yoon S, Bhandari A, Corominas R, Iakoucheva LM, Krastoshevsky O, Krause V, Larach-Walters V, Welsh DK, Craig D, Kelsoe JR, Gershon ES, Leal SM, Dell Aquila M, Morris DW, Gill M, Corvin A, Insel PA, McClellan J, King MC, Karayiorgou M, Levy DL, DeLisi LE, Sebat J: Duplications of the neuropeptide receptor gene VIPR2 confer significant risk for schizophrenia. Nature. 2011, 471: 499-503. 10.1038/nature09884.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Sebat J, Lakshmi B, Malhotra D, Troge J, Lese-Martin C, Walsh T, Yamrom B, Yoon S, Krasnitz A, Kendall J, Leotta A, Pai D, Zhang R, Lee YH, Hicks J, Spence SJ, Lee AT, Puura K, Lehtimäki T, Ledbetter D, Gregersen PK, Bregman J, Sutcliffe JS, Jobanputra V, Chung W, Warburton D, King MC, Skuse D, Geschwind DH, Gilliam TC, et al: Strong association of de novo copy number mutations with autism. Science. 2007, 316: 445-449. 10.1126/science.1138659.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Marshall CR, Noor A, Vincent JB, Lionel AC, Feuk L, Skaug J, Shago M, Moessner R, Pinto D, Ren Y, Thiruvahindrapduram B, Fiebig A, Schreiber S, Friedman J, Ketelaars CE, Vos YJ, Ficicioglu C, Kirkpatrick S, Nicolson R, Sloman L, Summers A, Gibbons CA, Teebi A, Chitayat D, Weksberg R, Thompson A, Vardy C, Crosbie V, Luscombe S, Baatjes R, et al: Structural variation of chromosomes in autism spectrum disorder. Am J Hum Genet. 2008, 82: 477-488. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.12.009.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Glessner JT, Wang K, Cai G, Korvatska O, Kim CE, Wood S, Zhang H, Estes A, Brune CW, Bradfield JP, Imielinski M, Frackelton EC, Reichert J, Crawford EL, Munson J, Sleiman PM, Chiavacci R, Annaiah K, Thomas K, Hou C, Glaberson W, Flory J, Otieno F, Garris M, Soorya L, Klei L, Piven J, Meyer KJ, Anagnostou E, Sakurai T, et al: Autism genome-wide copy number variation reveals ubiquitin and neuronal genes. Nature. 2009, 459: 569-573. 10.1038/nature07953.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. Sanders SJ, Ercan-Sencicek AG, Hus V, Luo R, Murtha MT, Moreno-De-Luca D, Chu SH, Moreau MP, Gupta AR, Thomson SA, Mason CE, Bilguvar K, Celestino-Soper PB, Choi M, Crawford EL, Davis L, Wright NR, Dhodapkar RM, DiCola M, DiLullo NM, Fernandez TV, Fielding-Singh V, Fishman DO, Frahm S, Garagaloyan R, Goh GS, Kammela S, Klei L, Lowe JK, Lund SC, et al: Multiple recurrent de novo CNVs, including duplications of the 7q11.23 Williams syndrome region, are strongly associated with autism. Neuron. 2011, 70: 863-885. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.002.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Elia J, Gai X, Xie HM, Perin JC, Geiger E, Glessner JT, D'arcy M, deBerardinis R, Frackelton E, Kim C, Lantieri F, Muganga BM, Wang L, Takeda T, Rappaport EF, Grant SF, Berrettini W, Devoto M, Shaikh TH, Hakonarson H, White PS: Rare structural variants found in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder are preferentially associated with neurodevelopmental genes. Mol Psychiatry. 2010, 15: 637-646. 10.1038/mp.2009.57.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Williams NM, Zaharieva I, Martin A, Langley K, Mantripragada K, Fossdal R, Stefansson H, Stefansson K, Magnusson P, Gudmundsson OO, Gustafsson O, Holmans P, Owen MJ, O'Donovan M, Thapar A: Rare chromosomal deletions and duplications in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet. 2010, 376: 1401-1408. 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)61109-9.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Lionel AC, Crosbie J, Barbosa N, Goodale T, Thiruvahindrapuram B, Rickaby J, Gazzellone M, Carson AR, Howe JL, Wang Z, Wei J, Stewart AF, Roberts R, McPherson R, Fiebig A, Franke A, Schreiber S, Zwaigenbaum L, Fernandez BA, Roberts W, Arnold PD, Szatmari P, Marshall CR, Schachar R, Scherer SW: Rare copy number variation discovery and cross-disorder comparisons identify risk genes for ADHD. Sci Transl Med. 2011, 3: 95ra75-10.1126/scitranslmed.3002464.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  84. Lesch KP, Selch S, Renner TJ, Jacob C, Nguyen TT, Hahn T, Romanos M, Walitza S, Shoichet S, Dempfle A, Heine M, Boreatti-Hümmer A, Romanos J, Gross-Lesch S, Zerlaut H, Wultsch T, Heinzel S, Fassnacht M, Fallgatter A, Allolio B, Schäfer H, Warnke A, Reif A, Ropers HH, Ullmann R: Genome-wide copy number variation analysis in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: association with neuropeptide Y gene dosage in an extended pedigree. Mol Psychiatry. 2011, 16: 491-503. 10.1038/mp.2010.29.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  85. Sundaram SK, Huq AM, Wilson BJ, Chugani HT: Tourette syndrome is associated with recurrent exonic copy number variants. Neurology. 2010, 74: 1583-1590. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181e0f147.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  86. Mefford HC, Muhle H, Ostertag P, von Spiczak S, Buysse K, Baker C, Franke A, Malafosse A, Genton P, Thomas P, Gurnett CA, Schreiber S, Bassuk AG, Guipponi M, Stephani U, Helbig I, Eichler EE: Genome-wide copy number variation in epilepsy: novel susceptibility loci in idiopathic generalized and focal epilepsies. PLoS Genet. 2010, 6: e1000962-10.1371/journal.pgen.1000962.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. Piton A, Gauthier J, Hamdan FF, Lafrenière RG, Yang Y, Henrion E, Laurent S, Noreau A, Thibodeau P, Karemera L, Spiegelman D, Kuku F, Duguay J, Destroismaisons L, Jolivet P, Côté M, Lachapelle K, Diallo O, Raymond A, Marineau C, Champagne N, Xiong L, Gaspar C, Rivière JB, Tarabeux J, Cossette P, Krebs MO, Rapoport JL, Addington A, Delisi LE, et al: Systematic resequencing of X-chromosome synaptic genes in autism spectrum disorder and schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry. 2011, 16: 867-880. 10.1038/mp.2010.54.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Vissers LE, de Ligt J, Gilissen C, Janssen I, Steehouwer M, de Vries P, van Lier B, Arts P, Wieskamp N, del Rosario M, van Bon BW, Hoischen A, de Vries BB, Brunner HG, Veltman JA: A de novo paradigm for mental retardation. Nat Genet. 2010, 42: 1109-1112. 10.1038/ng.712.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  89. Glessner JT, Reilly MP, Kim CE, Takahashi N, Albano A, Hou C, Bradfield JP, Zhang H, Sleiman PM, Flory JH, Imielinski M, Frackelton EC, Chiavacci R, Thomas KA, Garris M, Otieno FG, Davidson M, Weiser M, Reichenberg A, Davis KL, Friedman JI, Cappola TP, Margulies KB, Rader DJ, Grant SF, Buxbaum JD, Gur RE, Hakonarson H: Strong synaptic transmission impact by copy number variations in schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010, 107: 10584-10589. 10.1073/pnas.1000274107.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. Xu B, Roos JL, Dexheimer P, Boone B, Plummer B, Levy S, Gogos JA, Karayiorgou M: Exome sequencing supports a de novo mutational paradigm for schizophrenia. Nat Genet. 2011, 43: 864-868. 10.1038/ng.902.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  91. Girard SL, Gauthier J, Noreau A, Xiong L, Zhou S, Jouan L, Dionne-Laporte A, Spiegelman D, Henrion E, Diallo O, Thibodeau P, Bachand I, Bao JY, Tong AH, Lin CH, Millet B, Jaafari N, Joober R, Dion PA, Lok S, Krebs MO, Rouleau GA: Increased exonic de novo mutation rate in individuals with schizophrenia. Nat Genet. 2011, 43: 860-863. 10.1038/ng.886.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  92. Myers RA, Casals F, Gauthier J, Hamdan FF, Keebler J, Boyko AR, Bustamante CD, Piton AM, Spiegelman D, Henrion E, Zilversmit M, Hussin J, Quinlan J, Yang Y, Lafrenière RG, Griffing AR, Stone EA, Rouleau GA, Awadalla P: A population genetic approach to mapping neurological disorder genes using deep resequencing. PLoS Genet. 2011, 7: e1001318-10.1371/journal.pgen.1001318.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  93. O'Roak BJ, Deriziotis P, Lee C, Vives L, Schwartz JJ, Girirajan S, Karakoc E, Mackenzie AP, Ng SB, Baker C, Rieder MJ, Nickerson DA, Bernier R, Fisher SE, Shendure J, Eichler EE: Exome sequencing in sporadic autism spectrum disorders identifies severe de novo mutations. Nat Genet. 2011, 43: 585-589. 10.1038/ng.835.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  94. Bonnefond A, Durand E, Sand O, De Graeve F, Gallina S, Busiah K, Lobbens S, Simon A, Bellanné-Chantelot C, Létourneau L, Scharfmann R, Delplanque J, Sladek R, Polak M, Vaxillaire M, Froguel P: Molecular diagnosis of neonatal diabetes mellitus using next-generation sequencing of the whole exome. PLoS ONE. 2010, 5: e13630-10.1371/journal.pone.0013630.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  95. Ashley EA, Butte AJ, Wheeler MT, Chen R, Klein TE, Dewey FE, Dudley JT, Ormond KE, Pavlovic A, Morgan AA, Pushkarev D, Neff NF, Hudgins L, Gong L, Hodges LM, Berlin DS, Thorn CF, Sangkuhl K, Hebert JM, Woon M, Sagreiya H, Whaley R, Knowles JW, Chou MF, Thakuria JV, Rosenbaum AM, Zaranek AW, Church GM, Greely HT, Quake SR, Altman RB: Clinical assessment incorporating a personal genome. Lancet. 2010, 375: 1525-1535. 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)60452-7.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  96. Worthey EA, Mayer AN, Syverson GD, Helbling D, Bonacci BB, Decker B, Serpe JM, Dasu T, Tschannen MR, Veith RL, Basehore MJ, Broeckel U, Tomita-Mitchell A, Arca MJ, Casper JT, Margolis DA, Bick DP, Hessner MJ, Routes JM, Verbsky JW, Jacob HJ, Dimmock DP: Making a definitive diagnosis: successful clinical application of whole exome sequencing in a child with intractable inflammatory bowel disease. Genet Med. 2011, 13: 255-262. 10.1097/GIM.0b013e3182088158.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Gilissen C, Hoischen A, Brunner HG, Veltman JA: Unlocking Mendelian disease using exome sequencing. Genome Biol. 2011, 12: 228-10.1186/gb-2011-12-9-228.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  98. Chubb JE, Bradshaw NJ, Soares DC, Porteous DJ, Millar JK: The DISC locus in psychiatric illness. Mol Psychiatry. 2008, 13: 36-64. 10.1038/sj.mp.4002106.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  99. Friedman JI, Vrijenhoek T, Markx S, Janssen IM, van der Vliet WA, Faas BH, Knoers NV, Cahn W, Kahn RS, Edelmann L, Davis KL, Silverman JM, Brunner HG, van Kessel AG, Wijmenga C, Ophoff RA, Veltman JA: CNTNAP2 gene dosage variation is associated with schizophrenia and epilepsy. Mol Psychiatry. 2008, 13: 261-266. 10.1038/sj.mp.4002049.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  100. Mefford HC, Sharp AJ, Baker C, Itsara A, Jiang Z, Buysse K, Huang S, Maloney VK, Crolla JA, Baralle D, Collins A, Mercer C, Norga K, de Ravel T, Devriendt K, Bongers EM, de Leeuw N, Reardon W, Gimelli S, Bena F, Hennekam RC, Male A, Gaunt L, Clayton-Smith J, Simonic I, Park SM, Mehta SG, Nik-Zainal S, Woods CG, Firth HV, et al: Recurrent rearrangements of chromosome 1q21.1 and variable pediatric phenotypes. N Engl J Med. 2008, 359: 1685-1699. 10.1056/NEJMoa0805384.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  101. Sebat J, Levy DL, McCarthy SE: Rare structural variants in schizophrenia: one disorder, multiple mutations; one mutation, multiple disorders. Trends Genet. 2009, 25: 528-535. 10.1016/j.tig.2009.10.004.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  102. Bassett AS, Scherer SW, Brzustowicz LM: Copy number variations in schizophrenia: critical review and new perspectives on concepts of genetics and disease. Am J Psychiatry. 2010, 167: 899-914. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09071016.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  103. Ching MS, Shen Y, Tan WH, Jeste SS, Morrow EM, Chen X, Mukaddes NM, Yoo SY, Hanson E, Hundley R, Austin C, Becker RE, Berry GT, Driscoll K, Engle EC, Friedman S, Gusella JF, Hisama FM, Irons MB, Lafiosca T, LeClair E, Miller DT, Neessen M, Picker JD, Rappaport L, Rooney CM, Sarco DP, Stoler JM, Walsh CA, Wolff RR, et al: Deletions of NRXN1 (neurexin-1) predispose to a wide spectrum of developmental disorders. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2010, 153B: 937-947.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  104. Badano JL, Katsanis N: Beyond Mendel: an evolving view of human genetic disease transmission. Nat Rev Genet. 2002, 3: 779-789.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  105. Kearney JA: Genetic modifiers of neurological disease. Curr Opin Genet Dev. 2011, 21: 349-353. 10.1016/j.gde.2010.12.007.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  106. Funayama M, Li Y, Tsoi TH, Lam CW, Ohi T, Yazawa S, Uyama E, Djaldetti R, Melamed E, Yoshino H, Imamichi Y, Takashima H, Nishioka K, Sato K, Tomiyama H, Kubo S, Mizuno Y, Hattori N: Familial Parkinsonism with digenic parkin and PINK1 mutations. Mov Disord. 2008, 23: 1461-1465. 10.1002/mds.22143.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Bolszak M, Anttonen AK, Komulainen T, Hinttala R, Pakanen S, Sormunen R, Herva R, Lehesjoki AE, Majamaa K, Rantala H, Uusimaa J: Digenic mutations in severe myoclonic epilepsy of infancy. Epilepsy Res. 2009, 85: 300-304. 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2009.03.004.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  108. Girirajan S, Rosenfeld JA, Cooper GM, Antonacci F, Siswara P, Itsara A, Vives L, Walsh T, McCarthy SE, Baker C, Mefford HC, Kidd JM, Browning SR, Browning BL, Dickel DE, Levy DL, Ballif BC, Platky K, Farber DM, Gowans GC, Wetherbee JJ, Asamoah A, Weaver DD, Mark PR, Dickerson J, Garg BP, Ellingwood SA, Smith R, Banks VC, Smith W, et al: A recurrent 16p12.1 microdeletion supports a two-hit model for severe developmental delay. Nat Genet. 2010, 42: 203-209. 10.1038/ng.534.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  109. Harrison V, Connell L, Hayesmoore J, McParland J, Pike MG, Blair E: Compound heterozygous deletion of NRXN1 causing severe developmental delay with early onset epilepsy in two sisters. Am J Med Genet A. 2011, 155A: 2826-2831.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Roshal D, Glosser D, Zangaladze A: Parieto-occipital lobe epilepsy caused by a POLG1 compound heterozygous A467T/W748S genotype. Epilepsy Behav. 2011, 21: 206-210. 10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.03.003.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. van Swinderen B, Greenspan RJ: Flexibility in a gene network affecting a simple behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics. 2005, 169: 2151-2163. 10.1534/genetics.104.032631.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  112. Shao H, Burrage LC, Sinasac DS, Hill AE, Ernest SR, O'Brien W, Courtland HW, Jepsen KJ, Kirby A, Kulbokas EJ, Daly MJ, Broman KW, Lander ES, Nadeau JH: Genetic architecture of complex traits: large phenotypic effects and pervasive epistasis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008, 105: 19910-19914. 10.1073/pnas.0810388105.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  113. Zwarts L, Magwire MM, Carbone MA, Versteven M, Herteleer L, Anholt RR, Callaerts P, Mackay TF: Complex genetic architecture of Drosophila aggressive behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011, 108: 17070-17075. 10.1073/pnas.1113877108.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  114. Schaaf CP, Sabo A, Sakai Y, Crosby J, Muzny D, Hawes A, Lewis L, Akbar H, Varghese R, Boerwinkle E, Gibbs RA, Zoghbi HY: Oligogenic heterozygosity in individuals with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Hum Mol Genet. 2011, 20: 3366-3375. 10.1093/hmg/ddr243.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  115. Glasscock E, Qian J, Yoo JW, Noebels JL: Masking epilepsy by combining two epilepsy genes. Nat Neurosci. 2007, 10: 1554-1558. 10.1038/nn1999.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  116. Klassen T, Davis C, Goldman A, Burgess D, Chen T, Wheeler D, McPherson J, Bourquin T, Lewis L, Villasana D, Morgan M, Muzny D, Gibbs R, Noebels J: Exome sequencing of ion channel genes reveals complex profiles confounding personal risk assessment in epilepsy. Cell. 2011, 145: 1036-1048. 10.1016/j.cell.2011.05.025.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  117. Mitchell KJ: The genetics of brain wiring: from molecule to mind. PLoS Biol. 2007, 5: e113-10.1371/journal.pbio.0050113.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  118. Vissers LE, de Vries BB, Veltman JA: Genomic microarrays in mental retardation: from copy number variation to gene, from research to diagnosis. J Med Genet. 2010, 47: 289-297. 10.1136/jmg.2009.072942.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  119. Miller DT, Adam MP, Aradhya S, Biesecker LG, Brothman AR, Carter NP, Church DM, Crolla JA, Eichler EE, Epstein CJ, Faucett WA, Feuk L, Friedman JM, Hamosh A, Jackson L, Kaminsky EB, Kok K, Krantz ID, Kuhn RM, Lee C, Ostell JM, Rosenberg C, Scherer SW, Spinner NB, Stavropoulos DJ, Tepperberg JH, Thorland EC, Vermeesch JR, Waggoner DJ, Watson MS, et al: Consensus statement: chromosomal microarray is a first-tier clinical diagnostic test for individuals with developmental disabilities or congenital anomalies. Am J Hum Genet. 2010, 86: 749-764. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.04.006.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  120. Visscher PM, Goddard ME, Derks EM, Wray NR: Evidence-based psychiatric genetics, AKA the false dichotomy between common and rare variant hypotheses. Mol Psychiatry. 2011, doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.65

    Google Scholar 

  121. Chahal HS, Stals K, Unterländer M, Balding DJ, Thomas MG, Kumar AV, Besser GM, Atkinson AB, Morrison PJ, Howlett TA, Levy MJ, Orme SM, Akker SA, Abel RL, Grossman AB, Burger J, Ellard S, Korbonits M: AIP mutation in pituitary adenomas in the 18th century and today. N Engl J Med. 2011, 364: 43-50. 10.1056/NEJMoa1008020.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  122. Laederich MB, Horton WA: Achondroplasia: pathogenesis and implications for future treatment. Curr Opin Pediatr. 2010, 22: 516-523. 10.1097/MOP.0b013e32833b7a69.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  123. Klingseisen A, Jackson AP: Mechanisms and pathways of growth failure in primordial dwarfism. Genes Dev. 2011, 25: 2011-2024. 10.1101/gad.169037.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  124. Kerszberg M: Noise, delays, robustness, canalization and all that. Curr Opin Genet Dev. 2004, 14: 440-445. 10.1016/j.gde.2004.06.001.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  125. Kitano H: Biological robustness. Nat Rev Genet. 2004, 5: 826-837.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  126. Raj A, van Oudenaarden A: Nature, nurture, or chance: stochastic gene expression and its consequences. Cell. 2008, 135: 216-226. 10.1016/j.cell.2008.09.050.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  127. Eldar A, Elowitz MB: Functional roles for noise in genetic circuits. Nature. 2010, 467: 167-173. 10.1038/nature09326.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  128. Albert R, Jeong H, Barabasi AL: Error and attack tolerance of complex networks. Nature. 2000, 406: 378-382. 10.1038/35019019.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  129. Ruderfer DM, Kirov G, Chambert K, Moran JL, Owen MJ, O'Donovan MC, Sklar P, Purcell SM: A family-based study of common polygenic variation and risk of schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry. 2011, 16: 887-888. 10.1038/mp.2011.34.

    PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kevin J Mitchell.

Additional information

Competing interests

The author declares that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mitchell, K.J. What is complex about complex disorders?. Genome Biol 13, 237 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1186/gb-2012-13-1-237

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/gb-2012-13-1-237

Keywords